Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (7): 1133-1147.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01133
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
SONG Wenjing, CHEN Yixuan, HUANG Yunhui()
Received:
2021-08-29
Published:
2023-07-25
Online:
2023-04-21
Contact:
HUANG Yunhui
E-mail:yunhuihuang@nju.edu.cn
Supported by:
SONG Wenjing, CHEN Yixuan, HUANG Yunhui. (2023). Emphasizing recovery or improvement in charitable fundraising should depend on event controllability. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(7), 1133-1147.
Fundrasing information | Event controllability n (%) | Two sample z test (Ratio) (controllable vs. uncontrollable) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Unspecified | Controllable | Uncontrollable | ||
Study 1 | ||||
Recovery-description | 3 (3.6%) | 6 (7.1%) | 75 (89.3%) | z = ?10.65, p < 0.001, 95% CI: [?0.91, ?0.74] |
Improvement-description | 2 (1.3%) | 134 (87.0%) | 18 (11.7%) | z = 13.22, p < 0.001, 95% CI: [0.68, 0.83] |
Study 2 | ||||
Recovery-description | 9 (22.5%) | 11 (27.5%) | 20 (50.0%) | z = ?2.07, p = 0.039, 95% CI: [?0.43, ?0.02] |
Improvement-description | 12 (25.0%) | 28 (57.2%) | 8 (16.3%) | z = 4.22, p < 0.001, 95% CI: [0.24, 0.59] |
Table 1 Covariation of fundraising information type and event controllability
Fundrasing information | Event controllability n (%) | Two sample z test (Ratio) (controllable vs. uncontrollable) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Unspecified | Controllable | Uncontrollable | ||
Study 1 | ||||
Recovery-description | 3 (3.6%) | 6 (7.1%) | 75 (89.3%) | z = ?10.65, p < 0.001, 95% CI: [?0.91, ?0.74] |
Improvement-description | 2 (1.3%) | 134 (87.0%) | 18 (11.7%) | z = 13.22, p < 0.001, 95% CI: [0.68, 0.83] |
Study 2 | ||||
Recovery-description | 9 (22.5%) | 11 (27.5%) | 20 (50.0%) | z = ?2.07, p = 0.039, 95% CI: [?0.43, ?0.02] |
Improvement-description | 12 (25.0%) | 28 (57.2%) | 8 (16.3%) | z = 4.22, p < 0.001, 95% CI: [0.24, 0.59] |
Measurement content | Recovery-desription | Improvement-description | Paired sample t-tests | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
α | M (SD) | α | M (SD) | |||
Study 3a | ||||||
Perceived information types | Recovery level | 0.70 | 5.95 (0.70) | 0.91 | 3.60 (1.41) | t(79) = 13.08, p < 0.001 |
Improvement level | 0.92 | 5.29 (1.27) | 0.81 | 6.25 (0.64) | t(79) = ?6.16, p < 0.001 | |
Perceived function | Reduce loss | 0.75 | 5.80 (1.12) | 0.86 | 4.45 (1.48) | t(79) = 9.88, p < 0.001 |
Increasing gain | 0.82 | 5.79 (1.20) | 0.79 | 6.68 (0.95) | t(79) = ?6.84, p < 0.001 | |
Study 3b | ||||||
Perceived information types | Improvement level (vs. recovery level) | 0.83 | 3.76 (1.46) | 0.74 | 5.88 (0.67) | t(99) = ?13.10, p < 0.001 |
Perceived function | Increasing gain (vs. reduce loss) | 0.85 | 3.57 (1.51) | 0.81 | 5.93 (0.76) | t(99) = ?13.83, p < 0.001 |
Table 2 Manipulation check of fundraising information type and their impact on perceived function of charity projects
Measurement content | Recovery-desription | Improvement-description | Paired sample t-tests | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
α | M (SD) | α | M (SD) | |||
Study 3a | ||||||
Perceived information types | Recovery level | 0.70 | 5.95 (0.70) | 0.91 | 3.60 (1.41) | t(79) = 13.08, p < 0.001 |
Improvement level | 0.92 | 5.29 (1.27) | 0.81 | 6.25 (0.64) | t(79) = ?6.16, p < 0.001 | |
Perceived function | Reduce loss | 0.75 | 5.80 (1.12) | 0.86 | 4.45 (1.48) | t(79) = 9.88, p < 0.001 |
Increasing gain | 0.82 | 5.79 (1.20) | 0.79 | 6.68 (0.95) | t(79) = ?6.84, p < 0.001 | |
Study 3b | ||||||
Perceived information types | Improvement level (vs. recovery level) | 0.83 | 3.76 (1.46) | 0.74 | 5.88 (0.67) | t(99) = ?13.10, p < 0.001 |
Perceived function | Increasing gain (vs. reduce loss) | 0.85 | 3.57 (1.51) | 0.81 | 5.93 (0.76) | t(99) = ?13.83, p < 0.001 |
[1] |
Aaker, J. L., & Lee, A. Y. (2001). “I” seek pleasures and “we” avoid pains: The role of self-regulatory goals in information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(1), 33-49.
doi: 10.1086/321946 URL |
[2] |
Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556-574.
pmid: 10900996 |
[3] |
Ashraf, A. R., & Thongpapanl, N. T. (2015). Connecting with and converting shoppers into customers: Investigating the role of regulatory fit in the online customer’s decision-making process. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 32, 13-25.
doi: 10.1016/j.intmar.2015.09.004 URL |
[4] | Bateson, J. E. G. (1985). Self-service consumer: An exploratory study. Journal of Retailing, 61(3), 49-76. |
[5] | Batson, C. D., & Powell, A. A. (2003). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology (pp. 463-484). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. |
[6] |
Baum, A., & Fleming, I. (1993). Implications of psychological research on stress and technological accidents. American Psychologist, 48(6), 665-672.
pmid: 8328731 |
[7] |
Bhattacharjee, A., & Mogilner, C. (2014). Happiness from ordinary and extraordinary experiences. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(1), 1-17.
doi: 10.1086/674724 URL |
[8] |
Cesario, J., Grant, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Regulatory fit and persuasion: Transfer from “feeling right”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(3), 388-404.
pmid: 15008644 |
[9] |
Chang, C. T. (2011). Guilt appeals in cause-related marketing: The subversive roles of product type and donation magnitude. International Journal of Advertising, 30(4), 587-616.
doi: 10.2501/IJA-30-4-587-616 URL |
[10] |
Chang, C. T., & Lee, Y. K. (2009). Framing charity advertising: Influences of message framing, image valence, and temporal framing on a charitable appeal. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(12), 2910-2935.
doi: 10.1111/(ISSN)1559-1816 URL |
[11] | Cohen, J. (Eds). (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Routledge. |
[12] | Cohen, J. B., Pham, M. T., & Andrade, E. B. (2008). The nature and role of affect in consumer behavior. In C. P. Haugtvedt, P. Herr, & F. R. Kardes (Eds.), Handbook of consumer psychology (pp. 297-348). Erlbaum. |
[13] | Erlandsson, A., Nilsson, A., & Västfjäll, D. (2018). Attitudes and donation behavior when reading positive and negative charity appeals. Journal of Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing, 30(8), 1-31. |
[14] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/bf03193146 pmid: 17695343 |
[15] |
Feiler, D. C., Tost, L. P., & Grant, A. M. (2012). Mixed reasons, missed givings: The costs of blending egoistic and altruistic reasons in donation requests. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1322-1328.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.05.014 URL |
[16] |
Fisher, R. J., Vandenbosch, M., & Antia, K. D. (2008). An empathy-helping perspective on consumers’ responses to fund-raising appeals. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(3), 519-531.
doi: 10.1086/586909 URL |
[17] |
Friedrich, J., & McGuire, A. (2010). Individual differences in reasoning style as a moderator of the identifiable victim effect. Social Influence, 5(3), 182-201.
doi: 10.1080/15534511003707352 URL |
[18] |
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280-1300.
doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.12.1280 pmid: 9414606 |
[19] |
Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(1), 3-23.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992 URL |
[20] |
Hildebrand, D., DeMotta, Y., Sen, S., & Valenzuela, A. (2017). Consumer responses to corporate social responsibility (CSR) contribution type. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(4), 738-758.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucx063 URL |
[21] |
Huang, Y. H., Jia, Y. L., & Robert, S. W. J. (2017). The effects of physical distance from a brand extension on the impact of brand- extension fit. Psychology & Marketing, 34(1), 59-69.
doi: 10.1002/mar.20973 URL |
[22] |
Jain, S. P., Lindsey, C., Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2007). For better or for worse? Valenced comparative frames and regulatory focus. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 57-65.
doi: 10.1086/513046 URL |
[23] | Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L. & Thaler, R. H. (1991). The endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(1), 193-206. |
[24] |
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.
doi: 10.2307/1914185 URL |
[25] |
Kay, A. C., Whitson, J. A., Gaucher, D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Compensatory control: Achieving order through the mind, our institutions, and the heavens. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 264-268.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01649.x URL |
[26] |
Kidwell, B., Farmer, A., & Hardesty, D. M. (2013). Getting liberals and conservatives to go green: Political ideology and congruent appeals. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(2), 350-367.
doi: 10.1086/670610 URL |
[27] |
Kulow, K., & Kramer, T. (2016). In pursuit of good karma: When charitable appeals to do right go wrong. Journal of Consumer Research, 43(2), 334-353.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucw018 URL |
[28] |
Lavery, J. F., & Valerie, A. C. (1996). Causal attributions, coping strategies, and adjustment to breast cancer. Cancer Nursing, 19(1), 20-28.
pmid: 8904383 |
[29] |
Leys, C., Ley, C., Klein, O., Bernard, P., & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 764-766.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013 URL |
[30] | Liu, W. M. (2019). Having power, donating more time? The effect of donors’ power on their charitable donation intention of time and the underlying mechanisms. Nankai Business Review, 22(2), 23-32. |
[31] |
Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motivation by positive or negative role models: Regulatory focus determines who will best inspire us. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 854-864.
pmid: 12374440 |
[32] | McGuire, W. (1969). The Nature of attitudes and attitude change. In C. Lindezy & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed, pp. 136-314). Addison-Wesley. |
[33] |
Moran, N., & Bagchi, R. (2019). The power of emotional benefits: Examining the role of benefit focus on donation behavior. Journal of Advertising, 48(3), 1-8.
doi: 10.1080/00913367.2018.1556138 URL |
[34] |
Park, K., & Lee, S. S. (2015). The role of beneficiaries’ group identity in determining successful appeal strategies for charitable giving. Psychology & Marketing, 32(12), 1117-1132.
doi: 10.1002/mar.20852 URL |
[35] |
Ran, Y. X., Niu, Y. X., & Chen, S. Y. (2021). “More” is less: Why multiple payment mechanism impairs individual donation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(4), 413-430.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00413 |
[36] | Rothman, A. J., Desmarais, K. J., & Lenne, R. L. (2019). Moving from research on message framing to principles of message matching: The use of gain- and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior. Advances in Motivation Science, 7, 43-73. |
[37] |
Ryoo, Y., Sung, Y., & Chechelnytska, I. (2020). What makes materialistic consumers more ethical? Self-benefit vs. other-benefit appeals. Journal of Business Research, 110, 173-183.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.01.019 URL |
[38] |
Savary, J., Goldsmith, K., & Dhar, R. (2014). Giving against the odds: When tempting alternatives increase willingness to donate. Journal of Marketing Research, 52(1), 27-38.
doi: 10.1509/jmr.13.0244 URL |
[39] |
Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callousness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(2), 143-153.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.01.005 URL |
[40] |
Smith, I. H., Aquino, K., Koleva, S., & Graham, J. (2014). The moral ties that bind... even to out-groups: The interactive effect of moral identity and the binding moral foundations. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1554-1562.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614534450 URL |
[41] |
Steiger, J. H. (2004). Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(2), 164-182.
doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164 pmid: 15137887 |
[42] | Swanson, S. R., & Hsu, M. K. (2011). The effect of recovery locus attributions and service failure severity on word-of-mouth and repurchase behaviors in the hospitality industry. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 35(4), 511-529. |
[43] |
Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2003). To do or to have? That is the question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1193-1202.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1193 pmid: 14674824 |
[44] |
Vanhamme, J., Lindgreen, A., Reast, J., & Van Popering, N. (2012). To do well by doing good: Improving corporate image through cause- related marketing. Journal of Business Ethics, 109(3), 259-274.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-011-1134-0 URL |
[45] |
Wang, J., & Lee, A. Y. (2006). The role of regulatory focus in preference construction. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(1), 28-38.
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.43.1.28 URL |
[46] | Wang. J., Song. Q. Y., Xu. Y. P., Jia. B. B., & Hu. C. P. (2019). Calculating confidence intervals of Cohen’s d and Eta-squared: A practical primer. Psychology: Techniques and Applications, 7(5), 284-296. |
[47] | White, K., & Peloza, J. (2009). Self-benefit versus other-benefit marketing appeals: Their effectiveness in generating charitable support. Journal of Marketing, 73(4), 109-124. |
[48] |
Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322(5898), 115-117.
doi: 10.1126/science.1159845 pmid: 18832647 |
[49] |
Winterich, K., Mittal, V., & Aquino, K. (2013). When does recognition increase charitable behavior? Toward a moral identity-based model. Journal of Marketing, 77(3), 121-134.
doi: 10.1509/jm.11.0477 URL |
[50] |
Winterich, K. P., & Zhang, Y. (2014). Accepting inequality deters responsibility: How power distance decreases charitable behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(2), 274-293.
doi: 10.1086/675927 URL |
[51] |
Yang, W. Q., Li, Q., Guo, M. Y., Fan, Q., & He, Y. L. (2017). The effects of power on human behavior: The perspective of regulatory focus. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(3), 404-415.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00404 |
[52] |
Zagefka, H., & James, T. (2015). The psychology of charitable donations to disaster victims and beyond: The psychology of charitable donations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 9(1), 155-192.
doi: 10.1111/sipr.2015.9.issue-1 URL |
[53] |
Zheng, L., Zhu, Y., & Jiang, R. (2019). The mediating role of moral elevation in cause-related marketing: A moral psychological perspective. Journal of Business Ethics, 156(2), 439-454.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-017-3614-3 |
[1] | GENG Xiaowei, JIANG Hongyi. Influence of regulatory focus and regulatory fit on impact biases in affective forecast [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(12): 1537-1547. |
[2] | DUAN Jinyun;ZHOU Ran;LU Wenjuan;LI Jing;ZHU Yichao. The Impact of Regulatory Fit on Advice-Taking in Decision Making under Different Cues [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(1): 104-113. |
[3] | WANG Ling,LIN Hui-Yun,PANG Xiao-Ming. The Coincidence between the Regulatory Fit Effects Based on Chronic Regulatory Focus and Situational Regulatory Focus [J]. , 2011, 43(05): 553-560. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||